Imagine heading out for an easy jog, but with the feeling in your legs magically altered so that they burn with the pain you would normally experience at a much faster pace. Nothing else is affected: your heart rate remains low, your breathing is untroubled, your mind is sharp. How would this impact your ability to continue? Would you be able to keep going for as long as you normally can, or would the pain force you to stop early?
That鈥檚 the basic question posed in , from the research group of Alexis Mauger at the University of Kent in Britain. He induced heightened pain using an injection of hypertonic saline (water that鈥檚 saltier than blood) in the thigh, then tested the endurance of his subjects鈥 leg muscles. The basic result may seem obvious: the subjects quit sooner when they were in more pain. But the interesting question鈥攁nd the answer is聽not as obvious as it may seem鈥攊s: Why?
For a long time, I didn鈥檛 think much about the vocabulary I used to describe what the crux of a hard race or workout feels like. It鈥檚 difficult and painful and exhausting; you鈥檙e drowning in acid or piggybacking a bear or (my go-to) 鈥渞igging鈥 (to rig聽being the unofficial verb form of rigor mortis). But those words don鈥檛 all mean the same thing. Do you really stop because it hurts too much? Or is there something else that makes you incapable, or at least unwilling, to continue?
These are deep waters and difficult questions, which, once I started wondering about them, turned out to be so interesting that I ended up writing聽 about them a few years ago. But one distinction that鈥檚 much clearer to me now is the difference between effort, which researchers sometimes define as 鈥渢he struggle to continue against a mounting desire to stop,鈥澛燼nd pain, which, in the context of exercise, we can define as 鈥渢he conscious sensation of aching and burning in the active muscles.鈥
Back in 2015, I saw a conference presentation by a researcher named Walter Staiano that contrasted these two sensations. The data he presented that day was eventually . In one experiment, he and his colleagues asked volunteers to plunge their hands in ice water until they couldn鈥檛 tolerate it anymore, rating their pain on a scale from zero聽to ten聽every 30 seconds. As you鈥檇 expect, pain ratings climbed steadily until they approached the maximum value (peaking at 9.7,聽on average), at which point the volunteers gave up. In the ice-water test, pain is the limiting factor.
Then, with this experience of what ten-out-of-ten聽pain feels like, they performed a cycling test to exhaustion, rating both their pain and their sense of effort (on the Borg scale, which runs from 6 to 20) once per minute. As the study explains, 鈥減articipants were reminded not to mix up their ratings of the conscious sensation of how hard they were driving their legs (an important component of overall perception of effort during cycling) with the conscious sensation of aching and burning in their leg muscles (muscle pain).鈥
Which one is the limiting factor? As the cycling test progressed, both pain and effort drifted steadily upward. On average, by the time the subjects gave up, their pain rating was 5.0 out of 10. That corresponds to 鈥渟trong鈥 pain聽but is still a long way from the near maximal values they experienced in the ice-water test. Effort, on the other hand, got all the way to 19.6 out of 20 on average. It鈥檚 tempting to conclude that the subjects quit because their effort was maxed out.
Here鈥檚 what the data from the cycling test looks like. The pain ratings (RPU), shown on the left axis, are drawn with circles and a solid line; the effort ratings (RPE), shown on the right axis, are drawn with triangles and a dashed line. The horizontal axis shows the passage of time, scaled to the eventual point where each subject gave up.

Based on this experiment and others like it, I鈥檝e been converted to the view that your subjective perception of effort is more important than pain in dictating your limits. That doesn鈥檛 mean pain is irrelevant. There鈥檚 no doubt hard exercise hurts, and that pain may indirectly influence your performance. For example, Staiano and his colleagues suggest that coping with pain demands inhibitory control, a cognitive process that may fatigue your brain in ways that increase perception of effort. In this view, you don鈥檛 quit because the pain becomes intolerable, but the pain is one of several factors that pushes your effort to its tolerable limits.
Not everyone agrees, though. Mauger, a former colleague of Staiano鈥檚 at the University of Kent (Staiano has since moved to the University of Valencia, in Spain), has published a number of studies in recent years exploring the idea that pain itself can be a limiting factor in endurance. The main goal of his new study was to establish a protocol that would allow him to modify pain while keeping other factors like exercise intensity constant. You can鈥檛 just ask subjects to exercise while poking them with sticks or dipping their hands in ice water, because that鈥檚 not how we experience pain during exercise.
The good news is that hypertonic saline injections seem to work. The exercise protocol in the study was an isometric knee extension, which basically involves trying to straighten your knee against an immovable load. Comparing a heavy resistance (20 percent of maximum torque) to a light resistance (10 percent), with the addition of the saline injection, his 18 subjects couldn鈥檛 detect any qualitative differences in the pain they experienced. The injection made the light load hurt in the same way as the heavy load. This opens the door for some interesting future experiments in which researchers alter pain without changing any other physiological parameters, hopefully in realistic activities like cycling and running.
For now, the researchers compared three different variations of the knee-extension test, with subjects pushing against a 10 percent load until they couldn鈥檛 sustain it anymore, which typically took a little less than ten minutes: once with no injection (shown below with open circles), once with the painful injection of hypertonic saline (triangles),聽and once with a placebo injection of weaker saline that didn鈥檛 cause pain (closed circles).
The pain graph is fairly straightforward. The subjects report higher pain right from the start of the test, and it stays high. Eventually, everyone reaches a near max value of pain before giving up, but the hypertonic-saline group maxes out more quickly (448 seconds, on average), presumably because it聽started at a higher value. In comparison, it聽lasted 605 seconds with the placebo injection聽and 514 seconds with no injection.

From Mauger鈥檚 perspective, this looks like a smoking gun, showing that 鈥渕uscle pain has a direct impact on endurance performance.鈥 The theory is that the salt in the injection triggers feedback through certain nerve fibers known as group III/IV afferents鈥攖he same nerves during hard exercise. That鈥檚 why the sensation of pain mimics the feeling of harder exercise. Eventually, it reaches a point where the pain becomes intolerable, and you stop or slow down.
But how do we reconcile Mauger鈥檚 results with Staiano鈥檚? Mauger鈥檚 subjects only gave up when pain was maximal; Staiano鈥檚 subjects gave up when pain was just five聽out of ten. I suspect that has a lot to do with the choice of exercise protocol. Mauger鈥檚 subjects were sitting in a chair trying to straighten their right leg. They weren鈥檛 out of breath or even moving. Just as in the ice-water challenge, it鈥檚 not hard to believe that pain was one of the dominant sensations they felt. Staiano鈥檚 subjects, on the other hand, were cycling, with all the other feelings and sensations that entails. Most of what we do in real life looks more like cycling than leg straightening or ice-water challenges.
It鈥檚 also worth taking a look at how Mauger鈥檚 subjects rated their perception of effort. He doesn鈥檛 spend much time discussing it other than to note that there were no significant differences in perception of effort between the groups at any time point. This seems like a blow to Staiano鈥檚 suggestion that pain may influence endurance by increasing perception of effort. But take a look at the actual data for perception of effort (RPE, on a scale of 6 to 20):

As expected, effort increases steadily throughout the test. And while there鈥檚 no statistically significant difference, it certainly looks as though the hypertonic-saline group (the triangles) has higher effort ratings throughout the test. At exhaustion, the subjects are somewhere around 19 on the effort scale, which is pretty close to maxed out. The data in this study isn鈥檛 sufficiently detailed to answer the question one way or the other, but in my view, it doesn鈥檛 rule out the theory that pain matters mainly because it changes your sense of effort.
If, at this point, you have the sense that we鈥檙e trying to classify invisible angels on the head of a pin, that鈥檚 understandable. Something makes us slow down, whether we call it effort or pain. But for me, blaming pain for my inability to race faster never felt quite right. Sure, there were lots of times when I . But there were also times when I successfully ignored the pain, and yet I still eventually encountered the feeling that I couldn鈥檛 go any faster. So for now, I remain in Staiano鈥檚 camp鈥攊f only because that鈥檚 how I prefer to remember my glory days.
For more Sweat Science, join me on and , sign up for the , and check out my book .