When you really think about it, 鈥渂ecause it鈥檚 there鈥 is not a very satisfying answer. That was the motivation George Mallory to a New York Times reporter in 1923 before heading off on his tragic third听attempt to climb Mount Everest. Of course, Mallory had some reasonable听incentives, too鈥攔eaching the highest point in the world for the first time, eternal fame, and so on. People who鈥檝e followed in his footsteps also have their reasons. Even today, Everest summiters听earn decent bragging rights.
听
But none of that explains why many of us head anonymously into the mountains or run midpack marathons鈥攐r, for that matter, do Sudoku puzzles or buy hard-to-assemble Swedish furniture. That鈥檚 the riddle that in Trends in Cognitive Sciences, by University of Toronto psychologist and colleagues from Brown and Carnegie Mellon, explores. According to the prevailing models of cognitive psychology, neuroscience, and economics, we鈥檙e wired to minimize effort whenever possible. But sometimes, Inzlicht and his colleagues argue, we value experiences and outcomes precisely because they鈥檙e hard, not in spite of that. That difficulty听can add value, a surprise they call the effort paradox.
听
The classical view of effort is that 鈥渢oil and trouble,鈥 as Adam Smith called it in The Wealth of Nations back in 1776, subtracts from the value we assign to things. If you can purchase a coffee table that arrives in pieces with a bag of seemingly mismatched screws and some inscrutable pictographic instructions, or simply buy the same thing preassembled, economic theory predicts that you鈥檒l be willing to pay more for the latter. But studies of the 听have found that we actually value the coffee table we鈥檝e had to grapple with more highly.
听
Similar motivations seem to underlie much of what we do outdoors. Climbing a peak may offer fresh air and a view, but just getting to the top is rarely the goal for climbers these days. Instead, they seek out challenging routes. Similarly, runners may start out in the sport听motivated by the desire to get healthier, but by the time they progress to ultramarathons their underlying goals have shifted. The first 100-miler you run may be fueled by a Mallory-esque inclination to find out what鈥檚 on the other side. But the second one is probably fueled by something else. In both climbing and running, beyond a certain level of experience, the effort required seems to be part of the allure.
听
Inzlicht and his colleagues present several frameworks for why this might be. One is cognitive dissonance: if you do something that鈥檚 really hard for an outcome that you don鈥檛 consider particularly听valuable, you suffer an unpleasant disconnect that you assuage by convincing yourself that the outcome was valuable after all. If I worked so hard to get this, I must really like it,听you tell yourself. As a result, you end up liking things that require effort.
Climbing a peak may offer fresh air and a view, but just getting to the top is rarely the goal for climbers these days.
Interestingly, other species听display a fondness for rewards they鈥檝e expended effort to obtain. Rats like food more if they鈥檝e had to work for it. The effect shows up in locusts, too, which suggests that cognitive dissonance isn鈥檛 the whole story, since locusts don鈥檛 do a whole lot of introspecting about their motivations. Instead, it may be that rewards obtained from difficult tasks seem extra sweet because of the sharp contrast between the unpleasantness of working hard and the joy of achievement.
听
Both of these options assume that what we really value are the fruits of hard effort, rather than the effort itself. But that鈥檚 not necessarily the case. The theory of learned industriousness听assumes that over time听we learn that working hard leads to rewards, so we begin to value the effort itself, like Pavlov鈥檚 dogs salivating at the sound of a bell. Or we may simply be hard-wired to derive pleasure from full immersion in challenging tasks, as the notion of flow seems to suggest.
听
The paper听doesn鈥檛 reach any conclusions about these theories. Instead, its goal is to point out the听blind spot in current thinking about听motivation, choice, and decision-making: sometimes having to work for something makes it more valuable, not less. And it closes with a series of听questions that still need exploration, like why some people are drawn to effort more than others, and whether that trait can be trained or enhanced.
听
For me, Inzlicht鈥檚 paper recalls听a my wife, Lauren, and I took in 2012, along Tasmania鈥檚 rugged South Coast Track. What sets this trail apart from some of the island鈥檚 more well-known hiking routes is how absurdly unpleasant it is. The coastline is buffeted by nonstop winds from the Southern Ocean, and it rains 250 days a year. Even in midsummer, we were repeatedly pelted by hail so fierce that we had to take cover. Long stretches of trail disappeared under thigh-deep pools of mud crisscrossed by downed trees and surrounded by impenetrably dense, leech-infested scrub. Lauren and I have done a lot of hiking, sometimes in extremely tough conditions. This was the hardest we鈥檇听ever struggled.
听
The question we started to wrestle with after a few days was: Why? Sure, we liked the trail鈥檚 remoteness, and the landscape was beautiful in those rare moments when the fog lifted enough for us to see it. But we couldn鈥檛 shake the feeling that we鈥檇 been drawn to this ordeal precisely because of how grueling we鈥檇 heard it would be. I don鈥檛 know which of the theories听Inzlicht discusses applies here, but I鈥檓 glad he鈥檚 asking the question鈥攂ecause听six years later, that hike remains one of the highlights of my life, for reasons that I鈥檓 still not sure how to explain.听It鈥檚 nice to think that maybe I鈥檓 not crazy after all.
My new book, , with a foreword by Malcolm Gladwell, is now available! For more, join me on and , and sign up for the Sweat Science .